The first big study of daily rhythms in fruit flies outdoors doesn’t match some of the basic results from decades of lab tests.
Fruit
flies flittering in lab containers have revealed much about how light
can set the master molecular clock that ticks out a daily beat in living
organisms. Yet watching daily rhythms in fruit flies caged outdoors
reveals regular surges in activity not seen in the lab, says geneticist
Rodolfo Costa of the University of Padova in Italy. And certain patterns
of activity seen in the lab don’t show up in the real world, he and his
colleagues report online April 4 in Nature.
A major
difference, he says, is that the typical increase in fruit fly motion as
day dawns doesn’t seem to need a built-in clock in the real world.
Flies with genetic mutations that disable their biological clocks don’t
join in the usual laboratory bustle of activity before lights-on. Yet
outdoors they perk up and get moving just like clock-normal flies. “This
was something really unexpected,” Costa says.
“We are not saying
that everything that has been done until now is useless,” he adds. But
some of the assumptions based on laboratory experiments, he says, should
be expanded to account for behavior in nature.
“The new study
very nicely illustrates the risks of extrapolating from laboratory
studies to natural conditions,” says neuroscientist and chronobiologist
F. Rob Jackson of Tufts University School of Medicine in Boston.
Decades
of laboratory work suggest that many organisms — molds, cyanobacteria,
mice, plants, people and so on — share genetic mechanisms that create a
master 24-hour rhythm influencing cell processes.
Understanding what
drives this rhythm and what derails it holds promise for treating sleep
disorders, managing the stress of shift work and optimizing medical
treatments — as well as understanding how the rhythms of life evolved on
a periodic planet.
In indoor lab studies, when lights flip on in
the morning and flip off at night, fruit flies are active around dawn
and dusk. Genetically normal flies start fidgeting and moving about
several hours before lights-on and then settle down during the day for a
siesta. They get moving again as lights-off nears. Adding dim simulated
moonlight shifts some of their activity to the night.
To
translate laboratory work to outdoor experiments, Costa and his
colleagues set both natural and mutant flies in clear containers in
Padova, with automatic equipment to monitor their activity. For
comparison, collaborator Charalambos Kyriacou of the University of
Leicester in England set up a parallel fly outfit in his children’s old
outdoor playhouse.
Outdoor environments expose flies to a much
more complicated mix of cues. Light appears and dims gradually.
Intensity and color change. Temperature rises and falls.
Unlike in
the lab experiments, the scientists saw the outdoor flies go through an
afternoon burst of activity. Instead of being a dawn/dusk animal,
“fruit flies are diurnal,” Costa says. Also he saw no moonlight effect.
And in some cases, temperature apparently trumped light as a cue.
0 comments:
Post a Comment